ImageVerifierCode 换一换
格式:DOC , 页数:16 ,大小:67.50KB ,
资源ID:1145203      下载积分:10 金币
快捷下载
登录下载
邮箱/手机:
温馨提示:
快捷下载时,用户名和密码都是您填写的邮箱或者手机号,方便查询和重复下载(系统自动生成)。 如填写123,账号就是123,密码也是123。
特别说明:
请自助下载,系统不会自动发送文件的哦; 如果您已付费,想二次下载,请登录后访问:我的下载记录
支付方式: 支付宝    微信支付   
验证码:   换一换

加入VIP,免费下载
 

温馨提示:由于个人手机设置不同,如果发现不能下载,请复制以下地址【https://www.docduoduo.com/d-1145203.html】到电脑端继续下载(重复下载不扣费)。

已注册用户请登录:
账号:
密码:
验证码:   换一换
  忘记密码?
三方登录: 微信登录   QQ登录   微博登录 

下载须知

1: 本站所有资源如无特殊说明,都需要本地电脑安装OFFICE2007和PDF阅读器。
2: 试题试卷类文档,如果标题没有明确说明有答案则都视为没有答案,请知晓。
3: 文件的所有权益归上传用户所有。
4. 未经权益所有人同意不得将文件中的内容挪作商业或盈利用途。
5. 本站仅提供交流平台,并不能对任何下载内容负责。
6. 下载文件中如有侵权或不适当内容,请与我们联系,我们立即纠正。
7. 本站不保证下载资源的准确性、安全性和完整性, 同时也不承担用户因使用这些下载资源对自己和他人造成任何形式的伤害或损失。

版权提示 | 免责声明

本文(newton-wellesley.doc)为本站会员(天天快乐)主动上传,道客多多仅提供信息存储空间,仅对用户上传内容的表现方式做保护处理,对上载内容本身不做任何修改或编辑。 若此文所含内容侵犯了您的版权或隐私,请立即通知道客多多(发送邮件至docduoduo@163.com或直接QQ联系客服),我们立即给予删除!

newton-wellesley.doc

1、 papersdream.3rsNovember 7, 1996Corrected 10/30/98WE DO NOT DREAM OF THE 3 RS: IMPLICATIONS FOR THE NATURE OF DREAMING MENTATIONErnest HartmannDepartment of Psychiatry, Tufts University School of Medicine; Sleep Disorders Center, Newton-Wellesley HospitalKey Words: Dreaming, 3 “R“s, reading, writing

2、, arithmetic, connectionist netsRunning Head: No dream of 3 RsCorrespondence to: Ernest Hartmann, M.D.Professor of Psychiatry, Tufts School of Medicine27 Clark St., Newton, MA 02459page 1Summary: This report examines the extent to which dream recall which involves the “3 Rs“ (reading, writing, and a

3、rithmetic). Two separate studies were done. In the first study, two scorers rated, on a blind basis, a total of 400 written dream reports, available from five previous studies. There was perfect agreement between the two scorers. They agreed that there were no instances of reading, none of writing,

4、and one instance of probable calculating in the 456 dreams. The second study was a questionnaire survey. Complete responses were obtained from 240 frequent dreamers (who reported remembering a mean of seven dreams per week). The study examined in two ways the frequency of the “3 Rs“ in their recalle

5、d dreams. First, in answer to direct questions as to how frequently they dreamt about each activity, over 90% of the respondents reported that they “never“ or “hardly ever“ dreamt about each of four activities: reading, writing, typing, and calculating. In answers to other questions, this group repo

6、rted spending a mean of six hours per day engaged in these activities. Secondly, responses as to the relative prominence of six activities (walking; writing; talking with friends; reading; sexual activity; typing) in dreaming versus waking produced two clear groupings of activities. “Walking“, “talk

7、ing with friends“, and “sexual activity“ were each rated almost as prominent in dreaming as in waking whereas the second group consisting of “writing“, “reading“, and “typing“ were rated as far more prominent in waking than in dreaming. The two activity groups differed at p .0001. Thus, the 3 Rs app

8、ear to occur very infrequently in dreams. These findings are placed in a theoretical frame which suggests that dreaming (compared to waking) deals very little with serial activities characterized by “input rapid-processing output“ in which the neural nets function in a feed-forward mode. Rather, dre

9、aming may be characterized by relatively broad or loose connection making in which the nets function more in an autoassociative mode.page 2IntroductionThere is a widespread view that we can dream about everything and anything, that dreaming uses and recombines the entire contents of our memory syste

10、ms. Thus, Jung says, “Dreams may give expression to ineluctable truths, to philosophical pronouncement, illusions, wild fantasies, memories, plans, anticipations, irrational experiences, even telepathic visions, and heaven knows what besides.“ (Jung, 1933). A modern biological view that dreaming inv

11、olves random activation of material stored in the forebrain by brainstem activity (Hobson, McCarley, 1977) would also suggest that everything in our memory storage should appear in dreaming and that those of us with a lot of memories of reading, writing, etc. should certainly have dreams about such

12、activities. Likewise, the related view that dreams represent excess daytime material (junk) being discarded by the computer-like mind (Crick, Mitchison, 1983) would suggest that everything we experience and remember which has no great importance should appear in our dreams and thus our dreams should

13、 be full, for instance, of material we have read in newspapers and magazines.Any exceptions to the rule of “everything appears in dreams” may be helpful in elucidating the nature of dreaming mentation. The present study was designed to look into such a possible exception: anecdotally it appears that

14、 reading, writing and arithmetic (the “3 Rs“) common activities in our waking life play a very minor role in our dreams.More specifically, this study derives from a contemporary theory of dreaming (Hartmann, 1996; 1998) briefly summarized as follows: The mind can be seen as a complex net or (network

15、 of nets) consisting of a large number of simple units with varying connection weights between the units, which can be modeled roughly by connectionist net models as pioneered by McLelland and Rumelhart (1986). All that can happen during waking or during sleep is that activation flows along page 3th

16、e net, portions of the net are lit up and connections are made, strengthened, or weakened. However, there is a difference between dreaming and waking: In waking connections are made in a more-or-less direct, linear, serial fashion, remaining relatively close to pathways running from sensory input to

17、 motor output. In dreaming the net is not constrained in this way and connections are made in a broader, less serial and more parallel fashion, ranging farther from any connections to input and output. In this sense, dreaming involves the less structured or less “tightly woven“ portions of the net f

18、arther from input-output paths. Another less spatial way of picturing the difference is to consider the two basic types of “connectionist nets“ that have been modeled feed-forward nets and auto-associative nets (Figure 1). The suggestion is that in waking the net functions in a relatively more feed-

19、forward fashion, whereas in dreaming the net acts in a more auto-associative fashion. The theory then goes on to discuss that the broad connections are by no means random but are guided by the dominant emotional concern of the dreamer (Hartmann, 1998).On this model, then, relatively straightforward

20、serial activities with rapid input-output processing and a feed-forward form of processing, would be more consonant with the waking rather than the dreaming functioning of the net. For professionals, students, and white-collar workers, the activities of reading, writing or typing, and calculating ta

21、ke up a good deal of waking time, and are probably the clearest examples of activities involving rapid, serial, focused, feed-forward processing; therefore the model suggests that these might be relatively lacking in dreams.This is, in fact, consistent with an impression I have had in looking over a

22、 large number of my own dreams as well as laboratory and home dreams from a number of research studies. It is also consistent with a statement by Stephen LaBerge that lucid dreamers trying to read in their dreams have great difficulty in reading more than a word or two (LaBerge, 1985).page 4There is

23、 limited research work in the literature on the occurrence of reading, writing and arithmetic in dreams. Calvin Hall and his associates, in a number of studies involving content analysis of dreams, have reported on the frequency of various settings and activities in dreams (Hall, Van de Castle, 1966

24、). Though they do not discuss the 3 Rs specifically they have two categories that might include them. Under “settings,” they describe, “vocational settings,” as a rare setting for dreams, which in a sample of 1000 dreams (500 male, 500 female) occurred in 4.8% of mens dreams, and 5.2% of womens drea

25、ms. Under “activities,” they also have a subtype called “visual activities” which they define as including “a large number of words denoting visual activitiessee, notice, read, watch, peek, glance, view, inspect, and distinguish.” Their results indicate that overall 11.8% of male dreams and 12.4% of

26、 female dreams included such visual activities. Unfortunately, both the settings and activities categories are much broader than dreaming specifically of the 3 Rs, which are not investigated in themselves. Calvin Hall does provide one further hint. In a summary article reviewing his studies (Hall, 1

27、951), he states that “such common waking activities as typing, sewing, ironing, or fixing things were not represented at all in these 1000 dreams.” Strauch and Meier (1996) have collected and analyzed the content of 500 laboratory dreams as well as a large number of dreams collected at home. These a

28、uthors make no mention of the 3 Rs, nor do they mention activity and occupation categories which might include them.In this study the frequency of the 3 Rs in dreams was investigated in two separate ways. One involved a direct “content analysis” type of examination of actual written dreams from seve

29、ral different studies. This has the advantage of being objective, and gives an opportunity to check inter-rater reliability between scorers. On the other hand, it is time consuming and difficult to rate more than a few hundred dreams in this way. Furthermore, it is possible that a great portion of p

30、age 5dreaming may not appear in written reports, since the dreamer is pressed for time. Thus, it is possible that dreamers tend to write down only the most vivid or outstanding or unusual aspects of their dreams, and perhaps ordinary activities such as reading or writing might occur but simply not b

31、e mentioned in a typical written dream report.Thus the question was examined in a second way, using a questionnaire sent to a large number of good dream recallers, who were asked about whether they recall any 3 R activities in their dreams. This method is less objective, since one does not know exac

32、tly how many dreams or which dreams are being recalled and examined, but in a sense it casts a wider net: each of these dreamers might very well be able to recall many 10s or 100s of dreams, be able to review them, and be able to consider whether any reading, writing, etc. was done, even if the read

33、ing and writing had not played a very prominent role in any dream.Study 1: We obtained written dream reports from a number of different settings. In this study, a total of 456 dreams from five different studies were examined: 100 dreams from 100 male students made available by William Domhoff on-lin

34、e (Schneider, A., Domhoff, G.W., 1998); 67 laboratory dreams from 67 subjects, made available by Roussy (Roussy, et al., 1996); 68 dreams written down as part of an evaluation of 68 patients at a sleep disorder center (Hartmann, et al., 1998); 80 dreams from a recent student sample consisting of one

35、 “most recent dream” and one “dream that stands out” from each of 40 students (Kunzendorf, et al., 1997); and 141 written dreams from 115 students from a recent unpublished study by the author and associates. Except for the first group of dreams all samples were about half male and half female.Each

36、dream was read independently by two scorers (intelligent professionals and students, with no specific training in this task) who were given a sheet with three columns for scoring asking page 6them to score any instance of 1) reading or 2) writing word-for-word more than two or three words, or 3) cal

37、culating or arithmetic. Specifically, the scorers were given the following instructions: for reading, “Does the dream contain any instance of reading (a book, document, letter, etc.) which includes more than one or two actual words? Do not count simply seeing a sign with one or two words such as “No

38、 Exit” or “Dream Wonderland”; for writing, “Does the report contain any instance of writing, typing or word processing a series of wordsmore than two wordsDo not count writing or painting a brief sign. Do not count, for instance, “I was painting a sign that seemed to say Bach-fiends”; and for calcul

39、ating, “Any detailed description of doing an arithmetic problem, algebra problem, geometry problem or any more complex calculation in the dream.”Study 2: This was a questionnaire survey, sent to a group of good dream-recallers. A brief questionnaire, consisting of a single sheet printed on both side

40、s was mailed to all members of the Association for the Study of Dreams (ASD) residing in the USA; it turned out there were exactly 400 such members whose addresses were available at the time of the mailing. The questionnaire included two major questions about dreams. First, the respondent was asked

41、to estimate on a 5-point scale how frequently he or she recalled engaging in certain activities (reading,writing, typing, calculating) in dreams, and then asked how much waking time was spent per day on each of these activities. Second, the respondent was asked to estimate in turn the relative promi

42、nence of six activities walking, writing, talking with friends, reading, sexual activity, and typing in dreams as opposed to waking life. The three control items (walking, talking with friends, and sexual activity) were chosen as activities which were quite different from the 3 Rs but which also dif

43、fered greatly from each other in order to allow comparisons not only between the 3 Rs and the three other activities but also within the activity-groups which might help elucidate the findings. page 7The questionnaire also included questions about respondents age, sex, and dream recall frequency. Th

44、e data on the first question were simply tabulated and expressed as percentages; there are no statistical comparisons in these data. On the question of relative prominence of the six activities, the data representing six scores for each of the respondents were analyzed by pairwise t-tests.ResultsStu

45、dy 1: There was exact agreement between the two scorers on all 456 dreams, so there was no need for statistical measure of inter-rater correlations. Both scorers agreed that in the entire series, there were zero instances of reading, zero instances of writing or typing, and one instance of calculati

46、ng. On the single instance of calculating in the dream series, both scorers did score yes, but both placed a question mark and expressed some uncertainty as to whether it was definitely calculating. The relevant line in the dream read simply, “I was at my desk doing my math homework, when ” It is no

47、t certain whether calculations were actually done, or whether the dreamer simply sat at a desk with some math-related material in front of her.Study 2: 240 persons (60%) returned the questionnaire by the pre-established cut-off date, two months after the questionnaire had been sent out. The responde

48、nts were 68% female and 32% male with a mean age of 48 5 years. They were frequent dream recallers, as expected, reporting a recall of 6.8 5.8 dreams per week. Table 1 presents the results on the first question, tabulating the frequency in dreams of the four activities: reading, writing, typing, and

49、 calculating, as well as the amount of time spent on the same activities during waking. The results are quite similar across all four: For each about 90% or more of respondents (84%, 92%, 92%, 95%) answered that they dreamt either “never“ or “hardly page 8ever“ about it. Yet these subjects spent a great deal

本站链接:文库   一言   我酷   合作


客服QQ:2549714901微博号:道客多多官方知乎号:道客多多

经营许可证编号: 粤ICP备2021046453号世界地图

道客多多©版权所有2020-2025营业执照举报